REVISTA DE PSICOLOGIA -GEPU-
ISSN 2145-6569
IBSN 2145-6569-0-7

   
 
  Attachment Styles and Relationship Satisfaction
Attachment Styles and Relationship Satisfaction
  
 
 
  
 
 

Sheila Pintado & Andrea Mendoza

 

   

Universidad de las Americas Puebla  / México 


Descargar PDF



Sheila Pintado.
Ph.D. in Psychology by University of Valencia (Spain) and specialist in Clinical and Health Psychology. Currently, she is working as a full time professor in The University of Las Americas Puebla (UDLAP) and investigating as a part of the SNI (national system of investigators in Mexico).


Andrea Mendoza. Licenciada en Psicología en la Universidad de las Américas Puebla (UDLAP) y su interés principal se enfoca en la terapia familiar, estilos de apego y bienestar, realizando en 2014 una tesis relacionada con este tema y obteniendo la calificación máxima.  En 2015, se incorporó al Colegio Darlington de Puebla como encargada del Departamento Psicopedagógico de la escuela. Entre las actividades realizadas con este puesto se destaca la intervención en crisis, escuela para padres, taller de inteligencia emocional para niños, así como terapia individual. Actualmente, forma parte de la Dirección de Primaria del mismo colegio, estando a cargo de la coordinación académica y conductual de los alumnos, docentes y padres de familia de la escuela. 

 


Recibido: 14 de Marzo de 2016
Aprobado: 30 de Junio de 2016

Referencia Recomendada: Pintado, S., & Mendoza, A. (2016). Attachment styles and relationship satisfaction. Revista de Psicología GEPU, 7 (1), 157-168.  

Abstract: Satisfaction with relationship is related to many variables. One of these variables is the attachment style of people. The objective of this study was to evaluate the relation between relationship satisfaction and the different attachment styles. The sample consisted of 162 persons with age ranged between 18 and 72 years with an average of 37 years (D. T= 13,214). The instruments used were the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS) and the Adult Attachment Scale (AAS). Each individual completed self-reported questionnaires anonymously. Results indicate that there is a significant relation between these two variables (p< .001), and we could observed that secure attachment was related to the highest levels of satisfaction. 

Keywords: Attachment, Relationships, Satisfaction, Couple.

INTRODUCTION

For many years social psychologists have been questioned about romantic relationships and their functionality. Several research and hypotheses have emerged from these questions in order to be able to give some answers about the qualities to find a couple or to be able to establish a satisfactory relationship. However, there are only few theories that have been able to prove this topic empirically, one of them is the relationship between the romantic relationship and the attachment theory. Attachment theory refers to the universal human need to form close emotional ties and to turn to in times of stress or suffering. The research that later appeared, suggest that there is a correlation between the attachment style developed in childhood and certain important variables during adulthood.

The term “Attachment” was conceptualized as the affective connection between two individuals that provides them with a firm emotional foundation from which they can interact with the world. Characteristics of this type of relationship include supportiveness, trustworthiness, caring, and acceptance (Bowlby, 1969). Since the parents are the closest people a child has, they are the ones in charge of teaching them how a relationship has to be. The findings of Hazan and Shaver (1987) suggest that the opposite-sex parent may be used as a model for what heterosexual relationships are like or should be like, and what a person should expect from a romantic partner. Thus, although both parents may contribute to a person’s beliefs about him- or herself and the social world in general, the opposite-sex parent may play a special role in shaping beliefs and expectations central to heterosexual love relationships. Several studies have reported that the relative prevalence of the three attachment styles in adults is similar to that found in infancy (Feeney & Noller, 1990; Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Mikulincer, Florian & Tolmacz, 1990). 

Bowlby classifies attachment into three types; secure, anxious/ambivalent and avoidant. The typical securely attached infant was distressed when the mother left the room, was comforted by her return, and engaged in active exploration as long as she was present. Caregivers were judged to be consistently available and responsive to the needs of the child. In the Anxious/ambivalent attachment, the child manifest constant fear with the possible separation of the mother, giving behaviors of protest when she moves and holding her in an exaggerated manner. During the separation the infant does not support the consolation of the stranger. 

The typical caregiver of an anxious/ambivalent attached infant, exhibited inconsistent responsiveness to the infant’s signals, being sometimes unavailable or unresponsive and at other times intrusive. Anxious attached infants appeared both anxious and angry and were preoccupied with their caregivers to such a degree that it precluded exploration while playing. In the avoidant attachment, the child shows little resistance to the separation and avoids heavily any contact. When he or she is separated from the mother, easily admits the consolation of the strange. This defensive reaction and rejection is a way to protect them, because they no longer trust the object of attachment (Bowlby, 1989). These infants appeared not to be distressed by separations, avoided contact with their caregivers, and kept their attention directed toward the toys. Caregivers of avoidant-attached infants consistently deflected their infants’ bids for comfort, especially for close bodily contact. 

Hazan and Shaver (1994) were the first to used attachment theory as a framework for understanding adult love relationships. They argued that not only early relationships had an impact on the romantic relationships in adulthood; they also claimed that romantic love was also a process of attachment that shares important similarities with the attachment between the child and the caregiver. The authors pointed out that if the couple of a relationship in the adult life began to give the same functions and were able to satisfy the needs of emotional support and security that parents, who are the main responsible during childhood, then at some point the attachment will be transformed from parents toward the couple.

The evidence that supports these approaches is vast: a large number of studies have demonstrated the association between adult attachment and satisfaction, both in marriage as in unmarried couples (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Banse, 2004; Collins & Read, 1990; Feeney & Noller, 1990; Guzmán & Contreras, 2012; Hammond & Fletcher, 1991; Hartup & Rubin, 1986; Hartfield & Raspon, 1993; Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Hinde, 1979; Hinde & StevensonHinde, 1986; Hollist & Miller, 2005; Kirkpatrick & Hazan, 1994; Meyers & Landsberger, 2002; Ojeda, 2006; Ortiz, Gómez & Apodaca, 2002; Retana & Sánchez, 2008; Rivera, Cruz & Muñoz, 2011; Shaver & Rubenstein, 1980; Shaven & Hazan, 1988; Shaver, Hazan & Bradshaw, 1988; Simpson, 1990; Weiss, 1982; Weiss, 1986).

Lopez (1993) notes that it is in the relationship with the attachment figures where the child learns to communicate with others, learnsnforms of intimate contact (touching and being touched, embrace and be embraced, kissing and being kissed, look and be looked at, etc.) and systems of communication, which subsequently will be influenced in emotional relationships and sex. Kobak and Hazen (1991) studied marital quality among 40 couples and found higher levels of marital satisfaction in securely attached couples. Secure attachment also is predictive of successful conflict resolution (Kobak & Hazen), relationship independence, commitment, trust (Simpson, 1990), and positive emotions in marriage (Collins, 1996).

Comparing the kind of attachment to romantic relationships it can be said that when a person is attached to the style "Secure," he does it freely, maintains its independence, can take their own decisions, knows that a relationship takes commitment, intimacy, and expression of feelings, so this person take the time and effort (Bowlby, 1969). In the research of Hazan and Shaver (1987) the authors reported that people with a secure attachment described their love life as happier, friendly and full of trusted experiences. In addition they tended to have longer-term relationships. Studies on attachment patterns in adults have shown that people with secure attachment tend to live close relationships with joy and positive emotions, trust each other and generally accept their partner despite the flaws, and have more ability to solve interpersonal conflicts. It also has been reported in these people the highest levels of satisfaction, confidence and lower levels of unmet expectations of their partners (Feeney & Noller, 2001). These kinds of people show an attitude of peace, joy, happiness and even good communication when the partner is absence. They are people who tend to value herself, that are able to behave independently, without their decisions depending on other member of the dyad. Generally, they perceived a good level of satisfaction with their partner and with the kind of relationship they have built (Ojeda, 2007).

People with an “Avoidant” attachment tend to feel little valued or recognized by their partner. They are very susceptible, cautious, and avoid giving all for fear of leaving emotionally hurt and are always on the defensive. People with high avoidance described their relationships characterized by fear of closeness and frequent emotional ups and downs (Hazan & Shaver, 1987).  For this reason the people in the unsafe attachment enjoy less of their relationships. In another hand, Feeney and Noller (2001) found that people in the avoidant style were more likely to say they have never been in love, to not compromise and to have low ideals about love. In this kind of people, love is very often marked by the rejection of the intimacy, by the difficulty to depend on the other hand, a lack of confidence, emotional ups and downs and the difficulty in accepting the defects of the partner. When a person with this type of attachment gets involved in a relationship, unconsciously they try to get rid of it, and they look for real or imagined reasons that believe them concern; in fact, the person finds their reasons and they want to be right on them, in order to breakup the relationship (Rony, 1992).

Finally, if the person has an "Anxious/ambivalent" attachment is characterized by being scary of intimacy, retracted and evasive (Bartholomew, 1990), the way they act when they are jealous is with feelings of misery and distress, in addition to thoughts of disillusionment and insecurity in the disloyalty and possible attack by the partner causing anger and irritation to the lack of control in the relationship (Díaz-Loving et al., 1986).  People with greater anxiety in the attachment are more prone to jealousy, obsession or extreme sexual attraction (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). Their relationships are characterized by an obsessive concern by abandonment, desire union end and reciprocity, mistrust, jealousy and a greater vulnerability to loneliness (Brennan & Shaver, 1995; Freeney & Noller, 1990; Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Hendrick, Hendrick & Adler, 1988; Pistole, 1989).  This type of people will show in the presence or absence of the other member of the relationship an attitude of restlessness, and even at times of despair because there is the uncertainty of abandonment, physical or psychological loss of the figure of love. In such a way that they look for up to know everything their partner does and where they are every time (Ojeda, 2007). When it has formed an attachment style with anxiously behaviors and insecurities the relationships tend to be unstable, apprehensive, jealousy, with higher levels of anxiety, and a dependent or addictive love. 

According to data obtained by different research as we see before, people with a secure attachment shows the higher levels of satisfaction and involvement in a romantic relationship, while the subjects with anxious or avoidant attachment are recorded the lowest levels of satisfaction in couple relationships (Davila, Bradbury & Fincham, 1998; Jacob, 1999; Kirkpatrick & Davis, 1994; Kovac & Hazan, 1991; López et al., 1994; Myers, 2000; Rivera, 1999; Timm, 2000).

From the above, this research is aimed to study the relation between the levels of satisfaction in couple’s relationship according to the attachment style.

METHOD

Participants

The sample for this research consisted of 162 participants, 67% women (N=109) and 33% men (N= 53). The average age was 37 years (D. T=13,214), in which the minimum age recorded was 18 years and the maximum 72 years. One of the requirements was that all the participants currently had a romantic relationship. Participants were selected randomly through availability.

Measures

For general data, we created a questionnaire for purposes of the research, where we collected the most relevant demographic data; gender and age.

To measure the relationship satisfaction, we used the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS), created by Spanier in 1976. It is a questionnaire of 32 items that measure the degree of harmony or adjustment of a couple. The population for this questionnaire is for married and unmarried couples. This scale assesses one general grade from 0 to 150 that consists in 4 dimensions; consensus: degree of agreement between the couple, in important aspects of the relationship, cohesion: degree to which the couple is involved in joint activities, satisfaction: degree of satisfaction of the couple in relation to its present and its degree of commitment to continue with this relationship, expression of affection: degree to which the couple is satisfied with the expression of the affection within the relationship and with the satisfaction that is derived from the sexual relationship. Its format is of scale Liker, with score of 0 to 4 or 5, with the exception of two items that scored 0 to 1 and an item from 0 to 6.

José Moral de la Rubia currently validates the Dyadic Adjustment Scale in the Mexican population in 2009. Its internal consistency was high (α=. 93)

To evaluate the attachment style, we used the Adult Attachment Scale (AAS). This questionnaire was created by Collins and Read (1990) derived from the descriptions made by Hazan and Shaver (1990) and adapted and validated in Latin American population by (Tacon & Caldera, 2001). This scale consists of 18 items that the average of the results gets three different types of attachments; secure, avoidant and anxious/ambivalent. The scales range from 1 to 5, where 1 means "never" and 5 "always".  The items are divided into three factors of six items each: dependency, anxiety, and intimacy. The Cronbach's alpha was 0.75, 0.72 and 0.69 respectively. The factor dependency is defined when the individual feels that she or he can depend on others and believe that they will be there when you need them. The anxiety factor is the degree of anxiety or fear about being abandoned or not wanted. Finally, the factor of intimacy refers to the comfort that an individual feels with closeness or intimacy.

Collins and Read (1990) reported that when the scores of the dimensions of dependence and intimacy are higher than the scores of the dimension of anxiety, there is talk of a secure attachment style; when the scores of the three dimensions are smaller, there is an attachment style of avoidance, and when the scores of the dimension of anxiety are bigger than the dimensions of dependency and intimacy, there is an anxious ambivalent attachment style.

Procedure

In first place, this investigation obtained the support of the ethical committee corresponded to investigate with human beings.

Then, the participants were randomly selected following the requirements (must be at least 18 years old and a current relationship). This research was carried out at a unique moment managing the instruments mentioned above on an individual basis. We emphasized to all the participants the anonymous and confidentiality of their responses, and after they signed the informed consent, we applied the questionnaires based on self-report measurements.

Finally, we created a database to make a statistical analysis using the program Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), and specific calculations in this regard: Chi-square and Pearson's correlation coefficient.

RESULTS

The total number of people who had a secure attachment style was 128. What we observed was that 113 participants gained a good satisfaction in their relationship and obtained a type of secure attachment. It should be noted that 44 of these 113 participants came out with an idealized relationship. Only 15 people obtained a type of secure attachment and marked a conflictive relationship with their partner. The total number of people with an anxious attachment style was 9, which 4 of them had a conflictive relationship and 5 of them harmonious. In total there were 7 people that had an avoidant attachment style, of which 4 have a conflictive relationship and 3 harmonious (Table 1). (See chart 1 in PDF)
  
For purposes of this study, and given the results obtained we wanted to add a section on attachment style called "undifferentiated" in which there are specific participants whose results fell into a pattern of dependence on the couple, considerable anxiety, and a good percentage of intimacy to the couple, 18 people formed this group; 7 of them of them had a conflictive relationship, 8 harmonious, and 3 idealized.
 
About relation between relationship satisfaction and the attachment style, we could observe that there was significant, p< .001 (Table 2). In this sense, our results show that relationship satisfaction with partners depends on the people´s attachment style. (See chart 2 in PDF)
  
On another part, we assessed the total of Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS) with the scores obtained in the different aspects to measure the attachment style, with a Pearson correlation (see Table 3).  It was noted that the total DAS maintained a statistically significant relationship with the aspect of dependence and anxiety (r= .257 and r= -.245, respectively; both with a bilateral significance of .01).  However, no relations were observed with the aspect of intimacy. Therefore, a greater satisfaction of couple relates to less emotional dependency and with less anxiety. (See chart 3 in PDF)
 
As for the aspects that measure the type of attachment, we found a significant relation between dependence and anxiety and intimacy (r = -.276 and r = .224, respectively, both with a bilateral significance of .01). We also observed that anxiety was related to the intimacy factor (r = -.291; with a bilateral significance of .01). That means that people who obtained higher levels of dependence have less intimacy and higher levels of anxiety. These results show that when there are higher levels of anxiety there is lower intimacy (see Table 4). (See chart 4 in PDF)

DISCUSSION

The results obtained in this study allow us to confirm that the objective was reached, which we can prove that the participants of this study, which had a secure attachment style, have indicated higher levels of satisfaction with their partner. This finding is consistent with prior findings (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Collins & Read, 1990; Feeney & Noller, 1990; Guzmán & Contreras, 2012; Hammond & Fletcher, 1991; Hartup & Rubin, 1986; Hartfield & Raspon, 1993; Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Hinde, 1979; Hollist & Miller, 2005; Kirkpatrick & Hazan, 1994; Meyers & Landsberger, 2002; Ojeda, 2006; Ortiz, Gómez & Apodaca, 2002; Rivera, Cruz & Muñoz, 2011; Shaver, Hazan & Bradshaw, 1988; Simpson, 1990; Weiss, 1986).

At the beginning of this research we make an emphasis that we were going to asked our participants about their age and their gender so that we could discover if there was a factor that influences our results. However, it was discovered that there was no significant relation with age and sex. This means that it doesn’t matter the age of a person or if they are women or men, what it really relates with higher levels of satisfaction is the type of attachment. 

One of the main limitations we found in this study was the amount of male participants, as our sample was by availability the amount of men was lower than the women. We believe that it was a limitation since there is a lot of research that have shown that there is differences of gender speaking specifically of couple relationship, such as communication (Tannen, 1990), the tendency to engage in one night stand relationships (Skolnick, 1978), and their abilities to read the non-verbal language (Hall, 1978).

In this study we observed that of all the participants whose relationship was conflictive, 76.6 % were women. However, in this case you cannot say that there is a difference between men and women because the number of the total participants was not equitable. Hazan and Shaver (1994) indicate that without doubt human beings are born with a need to feel secure. However, in their article they say that mainly the domain of care and sexuality forms the differences between sexes. In general, women are more oriented to the caregiving and men toward sexuality.

Our recommendations for future research would be to give emphasis on the equitable number of participants by gender; it would be very interesting to discover if there are differences. We also recommend to do the research with the two members of the couple in order to be able to see if there are other factors. It has been found that no matter who of the two members of the couple manifest himself as the predominant Secure Attachment, their features to engage, intimidating in the relationship, permeate each other and interact with the other, favors a satisfactory relationship, because it is associated with more positive features that feeds an interpersonal relationship (Ojeda, 2006). 

We can conclude in this study that the attachment styles have a relation with the levels of satisfaction in a romantic relationship. 

REFERENCES 

Banse, R. (2004). Adult attachment and marital satisfaction: Evidence for dyadic configuration effects. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 21 (2), 273-282.
Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment perspective. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 147-178. 

Bartholomew, K. & Horowitz, L. (1991). Attachment styles among Young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226-244.

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Separation, Anxiety and Anger. Nueva Cork: Basic Books.

Bowlby, J. (1989). Una base segura: Aplicaciones clínicas de la teoría del apego. (1ª ed.). Argentina: Paidós.

Brennan, K.A. & Shaver, P. R. (1995). Dimensions of adult attachment, affect regulation, and romantic relationship functioning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 267-283.

Collins, N. (1996). Working models of attachment: Implications for explanation, emotion, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 810-832.

Collins, N. & Read, S., (1990). Adult Attachment, working models and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58 (4), 644-663.

Davila, J., Bradbury, T. & Fincham, F. (1998). Negative affectivity as a mediator of the association between adult attachment and marital satisfaction. Personal Relationships, 5(4), 467-484.

Díaz-Loving, R., Rivera, S. & Flores, M. (1986). Celos: Reacciones ante la posible pérdida de la pareja. La Psicología Social en México, 1, 386-391. 

Feeney, J. & Noller, P. (1990). Attachment Style as a predictor of adult romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28 (2), 281-291.

Guzmán, M. & Contreras, P. (2012). Estilos de apego en relaciones de pareja y su asociación con la satisfacción marital. Psykhe, 21 (1), 69-82.

Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 845-857.

Hammond, J. & Fletcher, G. (1991). Atachment styles and relationship satisfaction in the development of close relationships. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 20, 56-62.

Hartup, W. & Rubin, Z. (Eds.). (1986). Relationships and development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hatfield, E. & Raspon, R. (1993). Love, Sex and Intimacy: their psychology biology and history. USA: Harper Collins. Collage Publishers.

Hazan, C. & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic Love Conceptualized as an Attachment Process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 (3), 511-524.

Hazan, C. & Shaver, P. (1994). Attachment as an Organizational Framework for Research on Close Relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 5(1), 1-22.

Hendrick, S., Hendrick, C. & Adler, N. (1988). Romantic relationships: Love, satisfaction, and staying together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 980-988.

Hinde, R. & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1986). Relating childshood relatinoships to individual characteristics. In W. Hartup y Z. Rubins (Eds.), Relationships and development (pp. 27-49). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hinde, R. A. (1979). Towards understanding relationships. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

Hollist, C. & Miller, R. (2005). Perceptions of Attachment Style and Marital Quality in Midlife Marriage. Faculty Publications, Department of Child, Youth, and Family Studies. Paper 43.

Jacob, F. (1999). Continuities in support experience: The prediction of marital satisfaction, emotional well-being and distress from attachment style, perceived support and nonsupport in the context of personal strivings. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 59(8-B), 4466.

Kirkpatrick, L. & Hazan, C. (1994). Attachment styles and close relationships: A four-year prospective study. Personal Relationships, 1, 123-142.

Kirpatrick, L. & Davis, K. (1994). Attachment style, gender, and relationship stability: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 502-512.

Kobak, R. & Hazan, C. (1991). Attachment in marriage: Effects of security and accuracy of working models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 861-869.

López-Ibor, J. (1983). Biblioteca básica de la educación sexual. México: Editorial Universo.

López, F., Gómez-Zapiain, J., Apodaka, P., Delgado, M. & Marcos, C. (1994). Historia familiar y de apego, estilo educativo, empatía y estilo de apego actual, como mediadores del grado de satisfacción en las relaciones generales, la comunicación afectiva y la actividad sexual en la pareja. Cuadernos de Medicina Psicosomática, 28/29, 19-34.

Meyers, S. & Landsberger, S. (2002). Direct and indirect pathways between adult attachment style and marital satisfaction. Personal Relationships, 9, 159-172.

Mikulincer, M., Florian, V. & Tolmacz, R. (1990). Attachment styles and fear of personal death: A case study of affect regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 273-280. 

Myers, E. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: Emotion regulation strategies and relationship satisfaction. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 60(7-B), 3574.

Ojeda, A. (2006). Satisfacción marital en parejas migrantes: Un análisis por combinación de Estilos de Apego/Amor. RIDEP, 21(1), 23-45.

Ojeda, A. (2007). Evaluación de diferentes estilos de vínculos de pareja. Diferencias entre residentes (México DF) y Migrantes (EEUU). RIDEP, 24(2), 59-76.

Ortiz, M.J., Gómez, J. & Apodaca, P. (2002). Apego y satisfacción afectivo-sexual en la pareja. Psicothema, 14 (2), 469-475.

Pistole, C. (1989). Attachment in Adult Romantic Relationships: Style of conflict resolution and relationship satisfaction. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6, 505-510.

Retana, B. & Sánchez, R. (2008). El Papel de los Estilos de Apego y los Celos en la Asociación con el Amor adictivo. Psicología Iberoamericana, 16(1), 15-22.

Rivera, D. (1999). Adult attachment patterns and their relationship to marital satisfaction. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The sciences and engineering, 59(11-B), 6120.

Rivera, D., Cruz, C. & Muñoz, C. (2011). Satisfacción en las relaciones de pareja en la adultez emergente: El rol de apego, la intimidad y la depresión. Terapia Psicológica, 29 (1), 77-83.

Rony, J. (1992). Las pasiones. México: Publicaciones Cruz. 

Shaver, P. R. & Hazan, C. (1988). A biased overview of the study of love. Journal of social and personal relationships, 5, 473-501.

Shaver, P.R. & Rubenstein, C. (1980). Childhood attachment experience and adult loneliness. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol, 1, pp. 42-73). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Shaver, P.R., Hazan, C. & Bradshaw, D. (1988). Love as attachment: The integrations of three behavioral system. In R. Sternberg y M. Barnes (Eds.), The psychology of love (pp. 68-99). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. 

Simpson, J. (1990). Influence of Attachment Style son Romantic Relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 5, 971-980.

Skolnick, A. (1978). The intimate environment: Exploring marriage and the family (2nd ed). Boston: Little, Brown.

Spanier, G. (1976). Measuring Dyadic Adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38(1), 15-28.

Tacon, A. & Caldera, I. (2001). Attachment and Parental Correlates in Late Adolescent Mexican American Women. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 23 (1), 71-88.

Tannen, D. (1990). You just don’t understand: Women and men in conversation. New York: Morrow.

Timm, T. (2000). The effects of differentiation of self, adult attachment, and sexual communication on sexual and marital satisfaction: A path analysis. Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 60(11-A), 4199.

Weiss, R. S. (1982). Attachment in adult life. In C. Parkers y J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), The place of attachment in human behavior (pp. 171-184). New York: Basic Books.

Weiss, R. S. (1986). Continuities and transformations in social relationships from childhood to adulthood. In W. Hartup y Z. Rubing (Eds.), Relationships and development (pp. 95-110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

 


Convocatorias
 
Fecha Máxima de Recepción de
Trabajos: Hasta el 30 de Marzo 2024

Fecha Máxima de Recepción de CV para candidatos a Evaluadores pares:
Cerrada

Fecha Máxima de postulantes para Coordinadores de Distribución:
Cerrada

Fecha Máxima para organizaciones postulantes a ser Auspiciadores:
Hasta el 30 de Enero de 2024

Fecha Máxima para organizaciones postulantes a ser Patrocinadores:
Hasta el 30 de Abril de 2024
Indexaciones
 
Dialnet

Universidad del Valle - Biblioteca Digital

ProQuest

Biblat

CORE

LatinREV

ULBcible

Latindex

MIAR

Índice H

Discovery Service UNAM

SERIUNAM

Google Academico

Fuente Academica Premier EBSCO

UNC University Libraries

The Seattle Public Library

MLZ

VUB Library

Imbiomed

CIRC

SJR

Index Copernicus

IBT-CCG UNAM

IBT-SPUP U. PORTO

EZB

Fidmatch
Indicadores
 
Google Scholar
Varios
 

Certificacion de Web de Interes Sanitario de PortalesMedicos.com
Compruebelo aqui
 
Hoy habia 110 visitantes (131 clics a subpáginas) ¡Aqui en esta página!

Grupo Estudiantil y Profesional de Psicología Univalle – GEPU -
Correo electrónico: gepu@correounivalle.edu.co / Teléfono: +57 3186901373
Edificio D8 Quinto Piso (por la entrada diagonal a la cafetería de guaduales) - Ciudadela Universitaria Meléndez
Universidad del Valle - Cali, Colombia
©2024